Skip to main content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Marital Quality and Outcomes for Children and Adolescents: A Review of the Family Process Literature

Publication Date

 

Marital Quality and Parent-Adolescent Relationships

Marital Quality and Outcomes for Children and Adolescents: A Review of the Family Process Literature

May 2009

By: Randal D. Day, Brigham Young University; with Elizabeth Hair and Kristen Moore, Child Trends; Dennis K. Orthner, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; and Kelleen Kaye, NORC at the University of Chicago

Project Page:Relationship Strengths in Married Families

This report is available on the Internet at:http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/08/RelationshipStrengths/LitRev/

Printer Friendly Version in PDF Format (51 pages)Free PDF reader exit disclaimer

[ Skip Contents ]

Contents

Executive Summary

Chapters

Bibliography

Executive Summary

The overall purpose of this report is to show how marital quality, strengths, and/or interpersonal protective factors work to enhance the probability that children will do better among families where strengths are higher. We reviewed the research about parental marital quality and child outcomes and showed how those two constructs are connected. Studies indicate that conflict and parents' ability to resolve that conflict affect children's levels of anxiety and self esteem, and these effects can carry over into adulthood. The quality of the parental relationship often spills over into an adult's parenting behaviors, creating a second source of tension in the family. Negative effects of parental discord are found for both boys and girls.

This review focused on adolescent and young adult outcomes so articles on early childhood outcomes or grade school performance were not included. Much of the current research on marital quality and adolescent outcomes has been collected from small or localized sample populations. Comparisons are often cross-sectional, not longitudinal. And discussions of parental marital quality are mainly based on the parents' report of conflict or discord. Within the current review, several of the longitudinal studies drew from the National Survey of Families and Households and the authors of a number of the other longitudinal studies shared their data sources.

The Relationship Strengths in Married Families project, of which this review is a part, addresses several of the gaps identified in the research literature by focusing on the adolescents and young adults themselves and draws on a longitudinal, nationally representative data set with information from both the parents and their offspring.

[ Go to Contents ]

Introduction

Since 1960, the number of unmarried couples in America has increased by over 1100 percent (US Bureau of the Census, 2003). It has been estimated that nearly a quarter of unmarried people in the United States between the ages of 25 and 34 cohabit (Horwitz & White, 1998).  As these changes have occurred, considerable research has accumulated concluding that family break-up and single-parenthood have negative implications for children, adults, and families.  Rigorous studies have repeatedly demonstrated the negative effects of parental divorce on outcomes of children, even into adulthood (Glenn & Kramer, 1985; Zill, Morrison, & Coiro, 1993; Sandefur and McLanahan, 1994).

Notwithstanding this focus on examining the implications of family structure, comparatively little research has addressed a related and similarly important question  specifically, how do parental marital quality and conflict affect outcomes for children and adolescents in the millions of married couple families that stay married?  The following review of literature will examine the marital quality construct, including:  how it is usually assessed; how it is measured in the NLSY97, the specific data set used in this research project; how marital quality influences both positive and negative outcomes in children; and what is known about the implications of marital conflict and relationship quality on outcomes for children, without the additional effect of divorce.

Existing research shows that on average children in married-parent families are better off than children who are raised in other family configurations. (Amato 2001; Amato & Sobolewski 2001; McLanahan, et. al, 2005). However, we know relatively little about how the various kinds of relationship qualities enhance childrens well-being. In addition, researchers struggle to explain why marriages involving step-parents do not confer the same benefits to children as do first marriages (McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994; Hetherington & Kelly 2001).

This literature review is part of a research project to examine how marital quality and parent-child relationships affect outcomes for adolescents transitioning into adulthood.  One of the working hypotheses of the project is that when couples have increased levels of marital quality, their children are less likely to exhibit problem behaviors. A second hypothesis is that higher levels of marital quality (or strengths) contribute to childrens chances of success  that is, showing higher levels of community responsibility, more leadership skills, higher levels of social cognition, greater achievement in school, etc. (Moore & Lippman, 2005).

Some benefits of marriage are well-known, such as increased economic resources and family stability (see Waite & Gallagher, 2001 for a review). However, there has been less focus on how marriage and inner family life generally may affect parents interaction with each other and with their children and why marriage does not produce this same effect for step-parent families. Remarriage alleviates some of the financial problems associated with divorce, but it does not appear to ameliorate the long term deficits experienced by these children in their educational attainments, labor-force commitments, or likelihoods of unmarried births (Cherlin & Furstenburg, 1981; Goldschedider & Sassler, 2006; Carlson, 2006).

Research has demonstrated that families typically have a balance between relationship assets and liabilities or deficits (Orthner, Jones-Sanpei & Williamson, 2004). The strength of a relationship comes as much from its capital or assets as it does from challenges that the family may face. While much of the current literature focuses on how family deficits are related to negative child and youth outcomes (Pergamit, Huang & Lane, 2001), a strengths-based approach can serve as a critical foundation for building successful relationship interventions and marriage strengthening programs (Orthner, Jones-Sanpei & Williamson, 2003). Therefore, a key question of this project centers on the connection between marital quality (relationship strength) and outcomes for children. While the existence of that connection has been well-documented in past research, we know much less about how the attributes of stronger marital relationships combine to generate positive outcomes in children, produce protective factors, and provide sources of resiliency when crises occur.

We also seek to understand how marital relationship quality may directly influence the quality and strength of the parent-child subsystem dyad. In addition, we will examine how religiosity within family life operates as a particularly interesting and valuable kind of relationship strength across family structure and racial/ethnic and/or socio-economic condition.

Below we examine what is meant by marital quality/strength and propose a theoretical base from which our hypotheses are derived. Within that theoretical discussion we examine how research about marital quality has emerged and suggest why the connection between marital quality and child outcomes is strong. We also show the connections among the constructs of marital quality, marital or relationship strength, and resiliency.

[ Go to Contents ]

Measuring Marital Quality

There are few more enduring and researched topics in family science than the idea of marital satisfaction, marital quality, and/or marital happiness. In 1979, Spanier and Lewis published a chapter about marital quality in the family science handbook entitled Contemporary Theories about the Family (Burr, Hill, Nye, & Reiss,1979). They found that (by 1979) there were several hundred studies that dealt with martial quality. Given the scope of this research topic, we cannot hope to present a cogent history of this complicated research topic here. However, we can distill from several reviews what seem to be the more compelling ideas that have emerged.

First, the construct of marital quality (satisfaction, happiness, adjustment, integration) is clearly separate from the idea of marital success. Most authors would agree that marital success or marital status describes the degree to which a relationship is intact. That is, marital stability or status is not truly a dichotomous variable (married or divorced), but instead describes, a continuum of intactness. Indeed, when we assess this concept, researchers often ask about disintegration of a relationship and want to know if a couple has seriously talked about splitting up, or how many times a partner has moved out.

Measures of relationship quality, on the other hand, have less straight-forward conceptual clarity. Some researchers see marital quality as a subjective evaluation of one individuals assessment of the relationship, while others see this construct describing relational attributes, and yet others focus on a broad spectrum of behaviors that can be observed. For example, Lewis and Spanier (1979) declared that many researchers chose to assess marital quality as the subjective evaluation of a married couples relationship (p. 269). In that vein, Spanier (1976) published the most widely used measure of martial adjustment called the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (See Table 1). This scale is a multi- dimensional scale that favors the relational elements of the marriage dyad. The first 15 items asks the married person to rate their marriage on an agreement scale. For example: How much do you and your partner agree about handling family finances (scored on a six point scale). The remaining items ask the person to rate such things as how often they discuss divorcing or separating, do they leave the house following a fight, and if they regret getting married. Spaniers scale moved researchers away from asking the couple (or usually one member of the dyad) if he/she thought she was in a strong, viable relationship. Instead, Spaniers scale asks about behaviors and sources of agreement.

Table 1. Dyadic Adjustment Scale
  Always agree Most always agree Ocassionally disagree Frequently disagree Almost always disagree Always disagree
1. Handling family finances 5 4 3 3 1 0
2. Matters of recreation 5 4 3 2 1 0
3. Religious matters 5 4 3 2 1 0
4. Demonstration of affection 5 4 3 2 1 0
5. Friends 5 4 3 2 1 0
6. Sex Relations 5 4 3 2 1 0
7. Conventionality (correct or proper behavior 5 4 3 2 1 0
8. Philosophy of life 5 4 3 2 1 0
9. Ways of dealing with parents or in-laws 5 4 3 2 1 0
10. Aims, goals, and things believed important 5 4 3 2 1 0
11. Amount of time spent together 5 4 3 2 1 0
12. Making major decisions 5 4 3 2 1 0
13. Household tasks 5 4 3 2 1 0
14. Leisure time interests and activities 5 4 3 2 1 0
15. Career decisions 5 4 3 2 1 0
16. How often do you discuss or have you considered divorce, separation or terminating your relationship? 0 1 2 3 4 5
17. How often do you or your mate leave the house after a fight? 0 1 2 3 4 5
18. In general, how often do you think that things between you and your partner are going well? 5 4 3 2 1 0
19. Do you confide in your mate? 5 4 3 2 1 0
20. Do you ever regret that you married? (or lived together) 0 1 2 3 4 5
21. How often do you and your partner quarrel? 0 1 2 3 4 5
22. How often do you and your mate get on each others nerves? 0 1 2 3 4 5
From: Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and other dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 1528.

Another research approach emerged in the 1990s with regard to assessing marital quality. Norton and colleagues returned to the work of the 1940s and 1950s and re-established a more global measure of marital quality and steered away from behavior assessments of partner activity (See Table 2. The Norton Quality Marital Index). While there are a few items in the Quality Marital Index (QMI) that assess behavioral indicators or hint at interactional style, most of the items are global assessments and rate an individuals overall attitude toward the marriage and/or long term partnership.

Table 2. The Norton Quality Marital Index
  Factor Loadings
Variable Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Mean SD
We have a good marriage. .83 .24 .24 .22 6.22 1.16
My relationship with my partner is very stable. .78 .29 .24 .28 5.95 1.38
Our marriage is strong. .77 .20 .22 .24 6.08 1.32
My relationship with my partner makes me happy. .77 .24 .21 .37 6.05 1.25
I really feel like part of a team with my partner. .68 .30 .23 .22 5.75 1.53
[the degree of happiness, everything considered, in your marriage)" .69 .39 .22 .24 7.74 1.81
How often have you and your partner seriously discussed ending your relationship? -.61 -.24 -.14 .10 1.02 1.75
I have often seriously considered ending my relationship with my partner. -.65 -.28 -.30  .01 2.03 1.63
Of the major things in my relationship, I am satisfied with about [percentage of satisfaction] .57 .45 .21 .19 .79 .16
I often wish I had not married my partner. -.53 -.32 -.25 -.12 1.91 1.37
My partner and I are very similar in our attitudes toward most things. .48 .42 .30 .09 5.51 1.24
We will probably still be together [estimated time] .47 .11 .39 .03 6.83  .85
My partner and I engage in many outside interests together. .43 .27 .11 .29 4.85 1.53
My partner and I agree on minor things in our relationship, about [percentage of time of agreement]. .18 .75 .10 .17 .74 .18
My partner and I agree on the major things in our relationship about [percentage of time of agreement] .41 .65 .17 .13 .81 .18
Of the minor things in my relationship, I am satisfied with about [percentage of satisfaction] .26 .46 .18 .11 .73 .19
I am very committed to making my relationship last. .34 .19 .67 .16 6.40 1.06
Which of the following statements best describes how you feel about the future of your relationship? . -.24 -.21 -.64   -.26 1.74 2.10
Indicate how much energy you give to your relationship as compared to work and other outside activities. -.02 -.07 -.17 -.66 3.22 1.41
Indicate how much energy your partner gives to your relationship as compared to work and other outside activities.: -.25 -.19 -.06 -.51 3.26 1.50
From: Norton. R (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the dependent variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141-151.

Bradbury et al. (2000) states that this latter approach is stronger and more defensible in assessing marital quality, and that if one wants to measure the behavioral world of marital interaction then one should exclusively rely on observational indices of interaction. In similar fashion, Gottman & Notarius (2000) make the same argument, noting that during the last 20 years or so, there has been a significant increase in the number of studies published in prominent journals that focus on interactive, behavioral elements and qualities of relationships. According to these and many family science scholars, behavioral coding assessments by a disinterested third party (usually the researcher and/or his/her staff) provides a much more pure and un-managed view of relationship properties.

The data used in this project, the NLSY97, adopt the behavioral indicators tradition.  These data do not have an observational component and therefore rely on an assessment of marital quality (or strength) by asking the individuals in the family (each member of a couple and the adolescent) for their opinion about the parents behaviors.  While recognizing the value of the observational approach described above, we nonetheless recognize the NLSY97 data as one of the richest data sources combining measures on marital quality, adolescent outcomes and an extensive array of covariates.  In addition, the specific behavioral indicators allowed us to examine elements of both assets and deficits related to family functioning. The source for items used in the NLSY97 was the Iowa Youth and Family Project (IYFP) (Conger & Elder 1994) and are shown on Table 3. These six items map fairly well on a few of the items found in the DAS (especially the behavioral assessment features), but map less well on the QMI (see Table 3 for a comparison).

Table 3. Marital Quality as measured in the NLSY97, DAS and QMI
NLSY97: How often is he or she fair and willing to compromise when you have a disagreement?

DAS: Items 1- 15 are a compromise score on individual aspects of the relationship. The NLSY97 item shown here could be thought of as a summative score of the DAS items.

QMI:         I feel like part of a team.

                  My partner and I are similar in our attitudes toward most things.

                  My partner and I agree on minor (and major) things in our relationship  

NLSY97:  How often does he or she scream or yell at you when he or she is angry?

DAS:        How often do you or your spouse leave the house after a fight?

                  How often do you and your partner quarrel?

QMI:        Nothing similar

NLSY97: How often does he or she insult or criticize you or your ideas?

DAS:        How often do you and your mate get on each others nerves?

QMI:         Nothing similar     

NLSY97:  How often does he or she encourage or help you to do things that are

                  important to you?

DAS:        How often do you think thing are going well between you and your partner?

QMI:        Do you confide in your mate?

NLSY97:  How often does he or she blame you for his or her problems?

DAS:        Nothing similar

QMI:        Nothing similar

NLSY97:  How often does he or she express affection or love for you?

DAS:        Do you have disagreements about showing affection?

QMI:        Nothing similar

Key constructs usually assessed in the study of marital quality not included in the NSLY97 family process items:
  • Broad assessment of disagreement (or agreement) about key daily relationship tasks (such as how money is handled, religious difference, sexual relations, friends, philosophy of life, etc.)
  • How often we have considered divorce or separation
  • Do you regret having married this person
  • Any assessment within which the couple is asked to rate the quality of their marriage directly (i.e. Do we have a good marriage?; Is our relationship stable?; I feel like I am part of a team; I am committed to this relationship?

While the literature documents concerns over the use of a strong behaviorally based marital quality measure, the NLSY97 approach is viable if other cautions are exercised. First, Bradbury, Fincham, and Beach (2000) warn against mixing items in analysis that cross conceptual boundaries. That is, some construct items used in this area of research assess attitudes (e.g. We have a good relationship.) while others measure more behavioral elements of relationships (e.g. How often do you confide in your spouse?). Within analyses, they state, it is important to separate those two approaches. But it is, they claim, critical to have both types of measures.  That concern is reduced in the Relationship Strengths in Married Families project because the data used are exclusively behavioral, assessing six areas of action: compromising, screaming, criticizing, blaming, encouraging, and showing affection.

Bradbury, et al., also note that behavioral assessments, when carefully controlled in longitudinal settings, can serve as suitable proxies for general global measures of marital quality (see also Karney & Bradbury, 1997). Reassessing marital quality over time (if carefully grouped) can provide a window in the quality of a relationship. In the current project, our analyses follows this advice to the extent possible: we assess behavioral measures of marital quality over two time periods. While, the NLYS97 did not re-interview the parents in subsequent data collection waves, we do have assessments of behavioral elements of marital quality during later waves through the eyes of the teen.

Next, Bradbury and colleagues remind us that a satisfying, strong, resource-rich relationship is not merely the absence of dissatisfaction or the lack of conflict. As they note, Recent discussion of the defining features of a healthy marriage (Halford, Kelly, & Markman, 1997), continuing interest in the attributes of long-term satisfying relationships (Kaslow & Robinson, 1996)all point to a developing conception of marriage and marital quality in which the unique dimensions of dissatisfaction and . (conflict) are recognized (as separate processes) (Bradbury, Fincham, & Beach 2000 p. 973).  They strongly recommend that both negative and positive features be assessed but that analyses separate these conceptually and statistically (see Fincham, Beach, & Kemp-Fincham 1997). These authors clearly show that these two dimensions have different correlate capabilities and account for different variance levels in a variety of outcomes.  Therefore, our project here separates the items conceptually into two features of marital quality: marital strengths (compromising, encouraging, and showing affection) and conflict/strife/dissatisfaction (screaming, criticizing, and blaming).

Finally, Bradbury et al. (2000) note that whenever possible, measures of marital quality should be assessed longitudinally. They contend that the shape and slope of the trajectory of marital quality needs to be referenced and indexed to a base-line time. Only then can more accurate estimations of its potential impact assessed. Our research here follows that advice and took account of the extent to which parent-marital relationships and parent-adolescent relationships were sustained over time when developing the relationships profiles used in our analyses.

[ Go to Contents ]

Strengths Perspective and Marital Quality

Increasingly, family researchers are utilizing a strengths-based approach in their research, trying to understand why some families when faced with stressors become stronger, while others disintegrate.  What are the qualities and strengths found in resilient families and how are they established and maintained?  Ultimately, the goal is to foster positive relationships among family members rather than solely focusing on addressing negative outcomes.  Marital quality is significant and key protective factor that has the potential to have a high impact on child-based outcomes.

Risk factors are simply the description of the probability of a future event.  Individuals with risk factors for a certain outcome are more likely to experience that outcome than individuals without those risk factors.  The concept comes from fields such as epidemiology and the insurance industry, suggesting that individuals with higher cholesterol, for example, are at greater risk of developing heart disease than individuals with lower cholesterol.  Just like cholesterol, risk factors can have both environmental and genetic components.

Nonspecific risk factors such as child abuse, chronic family conflict, academic failure, poverty, racism, etc. elevate risk for a variety of conditions (Coie, Watt, West, Hawkins, & et.al., 1993; Fraser, Kirby, & Smokowski, 2004).  These nonspecific risk factors have led some researchers to argue that cumulative risk may be more important than problem-specific risk (Appleyard, Egeland, Van Dulmen, & Sroufe, 2005). Perhaps the number of risk factors is a better predictor of outcomes than path models based on specific risks (Gerard & Buehler, 2004).  However, even though cumulative risk may predict some developmental outcomes, understanding specific risk mechanisms is necessary to target prevention and intervention programs (Fraser et al., 2004).

Specific risk factors may be more salient at different times in the developmental process.  While by definition, risk factors are individual, they are also context-specific and could be thought of as nested in the context of family, school, neighborhood, and broader societal influences that both affect and are affected by individual factors (Fraser & Richman, 1999).  Risk factors also differ by gender, race, and ethnicity.  Risk factors can refer to a specific event or a risk chain, that is, an event that may initiate change that increases the risk of other events.  One example of a risk chain is the death of a parent, which may lead to a stress disorder, but also change family dynamics which may increase the risk for other outcomes (Capaldi & Stoolmiller, 1999).

Protective factors, on the other hand, are both internal and external resources that modify risk (Fraser et al., 2004).  They may compensate for risk by directly reducing a dysfunction or they may mediate risk chains (Coie et al., 1993), in addition to having a buffering effect of moderating the relationship among risk factors and potential outcomes (Fraser & Richman, 1999).  These protective factors may be individual characteristics, family factors, or extrafamilial conditions. Distinguishing between mediating and moderating effects in high-risk populations is difficult, if not impossible. The tie to our project here is that marital quality is a key and significant protective factor that has the potential to have high impact on child-based outcomes.

Similar to the idea of risk factors, resiliency comes from epidemiology and the risk of negative medical outcomes. Individuals at risk for specific events or risk chains who overcome the odds and achieve positive and unexpected outcomes have been called resilient.  Three aspects of resilience are: overcoming the odds, sustaining competence under pressure, and recovering from trauma (Fraser & Richman, 1999).  Unfortunately, there are many measurement problems with the concept of resiliency.  The key problem is that resiliency is something of a circular condition, where a resilient individual is defined as one that succeeds, or thrives, and a nonresilient individual does not.  Resiliency is context-dependent and there is no common, agreed upon measure of success  either statistically or conceptually (Fraser & Richman, 1999).

Several groups of family researchers have conducted research projects in the area of family strengths.  Many of these researchers have developed dimensions of family strengths and family processes  protective factors that may contribute to resiliency.  High scores on any of these measures do not guarantee a positive, or negative, outcome.  Rather, the risk of success, or failure, in a statistical sense, of a future event is increased.

Protective factors, in family research, are seen as having a moderating/mediating influence in circumstances of external stressors to the family and marital relationship. Examples of protective factors include communication, cohesion, flexibility, conflict resolution or problem-solving, support, togetherness, family time, clear expectations, shared core values, and commitment (Olson, 2000; Orthner, Jones-Sanpei, & Williamson, 2004; Seccombe, 2002; Silberberg, 2001). Factors discriminating between families with more successful outcomes and those with less successful outcomes are positive communication (Olson, 2000), problem solving and conflict management (Cox & Davis, 1999), companionship (Orthner, 1998), cohesion around values (Barber & Buehler, 1996), and social support (DePanfilis, 1996).

These protective factors are not qualities, but processes.  For example,  McCubbin, et.al. (1997) suggest that resilient families: experience stress; possess strengths that protect and assist them in recovering from negative experiences; benefit from and contribute to a network of relationships; seek to make meaning of and develop shared understanding of negative experiences; and when faced with crisis seek to restore order and balance to their lives.  In this way families protect family members by providing processes or behavioral strategies for working through stress and negative experiences, not just possessing attributes of strong families.  These processes develop over time in response to a familys specific context and development stage, and are evidenced by indicators such as supportive family ties and marital satisfaction, similar to the family strength attributes (Conger, 2002; Patterson, 2002).  

Taken together, these conceptualizations of family resilience and the factors associated with successful outcomes allow us to identify important aspects and processes of family relations.  We can interpret that a familys sense of cohesion, ability and willingness to communicate, and access to social support are important aspects and processes of family resilience (Orthner, Jones-Sanpei, & Williamson, 2002).

[ Go to Contents ]

Religion and/or Spirituality and Marital Quality

A key to understanding marital and/or relationship strength is religiosity. Many of the worlds religions place an emphasis on the importance of marriage and family. A familys religious beliefs can promote family bonds, particularly between parents and children. In a study of 867 families from the longitudinal Intergenerational Panel Study of Mothers and Children, mothers religious beliefs and practices were positively associated with reports of the parent-child relationship from both generations, suggesting that the extent to which individuals internalize religion can have implications for family relationships.(Pearce & Axinn, 1998). Religious beliefs, in a study of 486 families with adolescents from the Nonshared Environments Study, appeared to temper parenting styles, promoting social responsibility and conveying a familys values to the next generation (Gunnoe, Hetherington, & Reiss, 1999).

Results from a study of 97 couples in a Midwestern metropolitan area suggest that joint participation in religious activities is associated with better marital functioning, greater marital satisfaction, more perceived personal benefit from marriage, less marital conflict, and greater use of collaboration for wives and husbands (Mahoney, et al., 1999).  Higher levels of parental religiosity among 90 African American adolescents and their married parents in the rural South were associated with increased marital interaction, quality and support, and lower levels of conflict (Brody, Stoneman, Flor, & McCrary, 1994).  The view that marriage is sanctified by God may encourage greater effort to support and maintain the relationship (Mahoney, Pargament, Murray-Swank, & Murray-Swank, 2003). Conversely, disagreements about what a sanctified marriage looks like can contribute to increased conflict in the family, e.g., disputes over appropriate gender roles.

A study of 2,945 couples from the NSFH suggests that dissimilarities in religious attendance are positively associated with the overall frequency of conflicts in a marital relationship (Curtis & Ellison, 2002). Mens religious attendance in these data is inversely related to the overall frequency of disputes over housework, finances, and sex. The same pattern is not evident for womens attendance. The NSFH data suggest that theological differences between spouses are linked both with more frequent conflicts overall, and with disagreements over household labor and finances. Religious homogamy, in general, was found to have little effect on the frequency of marital disputes, with the exception of financial matters.

Recent research has noted that regular religious attendance is inversely associated with the perpetration of domestic violence (Ellison & Anderson, 2001). Religious attendance among couples, in this study of NSFH data, was associated with greater levels of social integration and support, reduced risks of alcohol or drug abuse, and a decreased likelihood of psychological problems. Other longitudinal research, however, suggests that increases in religious activity do not lead to improved marital relations, but that increases in marital interaction and happiness are associated with measurable increases in religiosity (Booth, Johnson, Branaman, & Sica, 1995). Thus, the association between religious activity and marital quality based on this study appears to be reciprocal. Unfortunately, while the relationship between religious views and marital quality is well documented, there is limited empirical research on how religiosity might mediate marital quality and conflict, and thus affect adolescent and young adult outcomes (Mahoney, 2005; Wilcox, 2002).

[ Go to Contents ]

Marital Quality as an Independent Variable

Typically, the research on marital quality focused on antecedents. Researchers assumed that marital quality was important and attempted to show what factors contributed to higher levels of marital quality, strength, or satisfaction (see Bradbury, et al. 2000 for a review of this literature). Even into the 1990s there were very few studies within which marital quality was placed in the theoretical position as an independent variable. Typically, when so placed, it has been used to predict individual well-being (depression, suicide, or other measures of individual well-being) or to predict marital success (i.e. stability). We bring this to the readers attention because the literature about marital relationship quality and its connection to childrens well-being is surprisingly scarce.

A refinement occurred in the 1990s when researchers began asking married partners about the inner workings of their relationships.  Previously it had been noted  that couples who rated their marriages as having higher quality were less likely to get divorced, but few researchers had focused specifically on the attributes of higher quality marriages and what it was about those relationships that seemed to matter to the well-being of the adults and/or the children. It was during this time of exploration into what made marriages work that the marital or family strengths and family resiliency approach came into use.

This theoretical orientation has it roots in Hills ABC-X model of family crisis (Hill 1949; Hill 1966).  The resources aspect of this model described the idea that when there was some type of stressor event (be it normative, non-normative, ordinary, extra-ordinary, chronic or acute), the consequences of that event were ameliorated when the resource base of the couple or family group were stronger. The most obvious example would be in the case where there was someone injured in a car accident. The blow of that stressor event on the functioning (long and short term) of the couple dyad and even the family as a whole could be softened or ameliorated by financial resources (e.g. insurance).  Additionally, it was reasoned that there were other strengths, resources, or attributes, that is, resiliency, that families could bring to an event (or series of events) that would, in like manner, improve the situation.

Resiliency is the ability of a person or group (such as family unit) to rebound following some type of stressor event. The speed and/or efficacy of that rebound was hypothesized to be directly associated with the bank of resources available.  These could include personal resources such as abilities, talents, or relationship acumen, or relational resources, such as internal and external social support, or material resources, such as financial assets, health insurance and earned income.  All of these combine to both protect individuals and families from the impact of stressor events but also, lift them from the disorganization created by such events. One of those strengths is found within marital dyads.

A couples resiliency in times of stress is closely tied to their resource base. And, a key resource in that base is the strength or quality of their relationship. For example, couples that are good decision makers create efficiencies in times of crisis and/or during times of smooth sailing. If they are experienced and efficient decision makers, goal-facilitating choices can be made more quicly resulting in goal attainment efficiency. In like manner, couples who trust each other will be more likely to generate better options and choices, allocate resources more effectively, and implement course correction strategies during a crisis. Couples with non-conflicted communication patterns are more likely to find solutions to problems more easily, implement corrective strategies more quickly, and pay attention to the emotional and physical needs of family members.. Conversely, couples who are mired in highly conflicted interactional patterns are more likely to send mixed and unclear messages, find themselves diverted in confrontations that are not problem specific, and will be less likely to venture untried solutions for fear of reprisal and criticisms (for review of this literature see Anderson & Sabatelli [2007]).

[ Go to Contents ]

Marital Quality and Adolescent Outcomes

Children should have the opportunity to grow up healthy, with the ability to learn, to provide for themselves and their families in the future, and to contribute to society. In order to achieve those goals, they need to be healthy, succeed in school, avoid participating in illegal activities, and minimize externalized and internalized problems as well as risky sexual activity.  The families in which they grow up influence these goals in a myriad of ways.  Families can reduce vulnerability by removing and reducing risk factors.  They can increase resiliency by adding protective factors.  While some nonfamily risk and protective factors may be mentioned in this review, the focus is on family strengths, and thus some environmental and individual risk factors are not included.

The primary health related risk factors are poor dietary habits, limited exercise and regular physical activity, and lack of primary healthcare (Simeonsson & Gray, 1994).  According to the National Center for Health Statistics, the top five health risk factors in the United States are: cigarette smoking, binge drinking and marijuana use among high school students, low physical activity, obesity, and untreated dental caries (National Center for Health Statistics, 2006). Factors associated with these health risks among youth include family history, family income, race, poor diet, physical inactivity, obesity, and smoking (National Center for Health Statistics, 2006). Parental stress, race, poverty, parental education, and family structure also contribute to health outcomes (Bauman, Silver, & Stein, 2006). Protective factors are represented by the absence of these risk conditions and the presence of supportive values and lifestyles that promote positive health outcomes.

One key element of the risk chain for several key adolescent outcomes is academic failure.  For example, academic failure is a precursor to unemployment (Laird, DeBell, & Chapman, 2006, Orthner et al., 2002; Orthner, D.K., Cook, P., Rose, R. & Randolph, K.A. (2002)), poverty and welfare dependency (Bowen, Desimone, & McKay, 1995), higher mortality rates, suicide, criminal behavior, and intravenous drug use (Dornbusch, Erickson, Laird, & Wong, 2001; Kasen, Cohen, & Brook, 1998; Laird et al., 2006; Obot, Hubbard, & Anthony, 1999). Family risk factors for school failure and dropout include single-parent families, large families, parent(s) who did not complete school, and a parent in jail (Hauser, Simmons, & Pager, 2000).  Other risk factors include poverty, racial or ethnic minority, urban and regional location, prior academic failure, grade retention (Kaplan & Peck, 1997), delinquency, residential mobility (Tucker, Marx, & Long, 1998), and pregnancy.  Family protective factors, on the other hand, include support from adults  and parental involvement (Demaray & Malecki, 2003; Richman, Rosenfeld, & Bowen, 1998).  Stable, close, and caring relationships between parent and child play a vital role in developing resiliency (Dornbusch et al., 2001; Werner, 1990).  Other protective factors include involved teachers and community adults (Brewster & Bowen, 2004; Rosenfeld, Richman, & Bowen, 2000, Orthner, 2007), and involvement in extracurricular activities (Mahoney, 2000).

Alcohol and drug use are becoming increasingly common among adolescents. Family factors that increase the risk of adolescent alcohol and drug use include parental conflict, poor parent-child bonding, poor family management practices, poor family communication (Feldstein & Miller, 2006; Fergusson & Horwood, 1999), larger family size (Reinherz et al., 2000), and family alcohol and drug use (Biederman, Faraone, Monuteaux, & Feighner, 2000; S. Y. Hill, Shen, Lowers, & Locke, 2000). Non-family risk factors include school failure, association with drug-using peers, delinquent behavior (Fergusson & Horwood, 1999; Hallfors, Hyunsan, Brodish, Flewelling, & Khatapoush, 2006), and residential mobility (Felner, Primavera, & Cauce, 1981; Nation & Heflinger, 2006). There may also be different risk profiles for individuals based on ethnic/racial differences (Vega, Zimmerman, Warheit, Apospori, & Gil, 1993). Family protective factors include a small family, low parental conflict, caring relationships with siblings (Werner, 1994), parental monitoring (Barnes, Hoffman, Welte, Farrell, & Dintcheff, 2006), family involvement (Wang, Matthew, Bellamy, & James, 2005), and parental attachment (Feldstein & Miller, 2006; Kumpfer & Alvarado, 2003).

Externalizing behaviors such as juvenile delinquency and conduct disorder are pressing concerns facing U.S. society today.  Juvenile criminal arrests for both property and violent crimes peaked in the 1990s, and have since decreased (Williams, Ayers, Van Dorn, & Arthur, 2004, p. 212). Children with externalizing behaviors frequently come from families experiencing high level of stress (Appleyard et al., 2005; Dahinten, Shapka, & Willms, 2007), families with alcoholics or criminals (West & Prinz, 1987), or families where other members exhibit pathologies (Jaffee, Moffitt, Caspi, & Taylor, 2003; Pfiffner, McBurnett, Rathouz, & Judice, 2005).  Parent-child interactions and parent management practices are perhaps the most significant family risk factors for externalizing behaviors (Appleyard et al., 2005; Loeber et al., 2005; Pfiffner et al., 2005).  Family instability due to divorce also increases the risk of delinquent behavior  (Loeber et al., 2005).  Discipline practices characterized by inconsistent and highly punitive actions may increase both alienation and deviant behavior (Pfiffner et al., 2005).   Family protective factors include good parenting skills and family management styles (Short & Brokaw, 1994).  Protective family factors include (1) supportive parent-child relationship and family environment, (2) positive discipline techniques, (3) monitoring and supervision, and (4) family advocacy (Williams et al., 2004) as well as general social support (Maschi, 2006).

Internalized outcomes such as depression and suicide are influenced by family risk factors such as family psychopathology, parental depression (Marmorstein & Iacono, 2004), physical/sexual abuse, family conflict, having a nonresidential parent, low parental monitoring, and poor communication between youth and parents (Eberhart & Hammen, 2006).  The emotional climate in families with depressed children is less cohesive, less emotionally expressive, more hostile, more critical (Asarnow, Tompson, Hamilton, Goldstein, & Guthrie, 1994; Hamilton & Asarnow, 1997), less accepting, more conflictual, and more disorganized than in families without depressed children (Hammen & Rudolph, 2003). Although these studies indicate a correlation between the risk factors and the outcomes, the studies often did not indicate whether the risk factor preceded the outcome, thus a causal relationship is not verified (Wagner, 1997).  Children who live in poor, high-crime, urban neighborhoods were protected from depression by higher parental education, employment, and good health (Durant, Getts, Cadenhead, Emans, & Woods, 1995).

Factors increasing the odds of risky sexual behavior, leading to pregnancy and sexually transmitted diseases, are related to the proximity of others who are also engaging in risky sexual behaviors (Rounds, 2004).  For example, family norms that accept early initiation of sexual intercourse and intercourse with multiple partners and older siblings who are sexually active (East, 1996; Widmer, 1997), both increase the odds of risky sexual behavior.  Poor parent-youth relationships, communication, and parental monitoring are also family risk factors (Meschke, Bartholomae, & Zentall, 2000).  Factors associated with the early onset of intercourse include lower levels of maternal education, lack of religious activity engagement, early age at menarche, and family instability when the adolescent is age 14 (Cates, 1991); poor academic performance (Santelli & Beilenson, 1992); and drug and alcohol use (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996).  Adolescents from intact families tend to initiate sexual intercourse later than those from non-intact families, as well as hold perceptions of less risky sexual behaviors among their peers and friends.  Adolescents who have been sexually abused are also more likely to engage in risky sexual activity (Miller, Monson, & Norton, 1995).  Risk factors for adolescent pregnancy include family conflict, family management skills (Corcoran, Franklin, & Bennett, 2000), poor education/dropout, low job skills, single parent status, poverty, permissive sexual attitudes, experience with physical abuse, and living in a high-risk environment (Franklin, Corcoran, & Harris, 2004; Miller, 2002). Research on factors that protect adolescents from engaging in risky sexual behaviors is only beginning to emerge, generally through research on the absence of these specific risk factors (Rounds, 2004).  However, some studies indicate that positive sibling relationships may mediate family risks such as welfare receipt and teenage pregnancies (East & Khoo, 2005; Kolburn Kowal & Blinn-Pike, 2004).

Poverty and racial or ethnic minority status have unique risk and protective factors for many of the adolescent outcomes discussed in the literature.  Some of the family protective factors for African American youth include strong kinship bonds, strong work orientation, adaptability of family roles, strong achievement orientation, and strong religious orientation.  The odds of African American males succeeding in school are greatly increased by specific parenting practices such as emphasizing cultural heritage and personal power to succeed (Mandara, 2006).  Table 4 summarizes the commonly used risk and protective factors for selected domains and the variables used to measure them.

Table 4. Defining Risk and Protective Factors.
  Risk Factors Protective Factors Typical Variable Selection
Common Child maltreatment Interparental conflict Parental psycopathology Harsh, inconsistent parenting Positive parent-child relationship Effective parenting  
Health Poor dietary habits Limited regular exercise/physical activity Lack of primary health care Nutritious eating habits Regular physical activity/exercise Regular medical and dental care Smoking Eating with family
Academic Race/ethnicity Poverty Parent dropout Large families Single parent families Parent/child relationship Other adult relationship Race Poverty HH size Parental status Academic scores
Illegal drug/alcohol use Parental conflict Poor parent-child bonding Poor communication Poor family management practices Family substance abuse Small family (4) Low parental conflict Caring sibling relationships Parental attachment Family size Parent/child relationships Communication Drug use Alcohol use Sibling relationships Parental conflict
Externalizing behavior Family stress Family substance abuse Family pathologies Inconsistent behavior expectations Inconsistent parenting Low monitoring Poor family management Good parenting skills Good family management Supportive parent/child relationship Family advocacy Monitoring Hang out Movies Parent/child relationship Adult activities? School act up?
Internalizing behaviors Family psychopathology Hostility Critical Parental depression Less emotional expression Disorganized Poverty Parental education Employment Good health Lack of risk factors Parent education Parent/child interactions Confiding Income Depression?
Risky sexual behavior & teenage pregnancy Family norms Poor parent/youth relationship Communication Poor parental monitoring Maternal education Degree of religious affiliation Family income Family instability Family conflict Family structure Academic performance Drug & alcohol use Sexual abuse Physical abuse Intact family Parent/youth relationship Communication Monitoring Income Family structure Academic performance Substance abuse Sexual activity

[ Go to Contents ]

Marital Quality and Conflict: Implications for Adolescent and Young Adult Outcomes

Research suggests that the experience of parental marital conflict often repeats in the relationships formed by adult children. Over 30 years ago, research indicated that a couples current marital problems may be part of a specific familial behavior patterning; that is, current marital complaints were associated with the quality of their own parents marriages (Overall, Henry, & Woodward, 1974).  Recent research has produced similar findings, indicating that parents attitudes towards cohabitation and divorce, as well as premarital sex, are strongly linked to those same attitudes among their young adult children (Cunningham and Thornton, 2006). The results of such studies suggest that a couples high quality, low conflict marriage can influence their children to act similarly and to view marriage positively.

Only a handful of studies have approached the question of how marital quality affects children in married couple families and fewer still have focused on the effects on adolescents or young adults; however, studies conducted among married couple families consistently find that children are affected by what happens in their parents marital relationships. In a study of 79 married couples with an eldest child between 7 and 11, the ability of parents to resolve their conflicts successfully was associated with self-reported levels of anxiety in children (Kerig, 1996). That is, parents who more constructively resolved their conflicts had children who reported lower levels of anxiety. A study of 146 sixth and seventh graders found that their perceptions of marital conflict both directly and indirectly affected internalizing behaviors, and also indirectly affected externalizing behaviors, while a companion study of 451 families found that perceptions of parental marital conflict were associated with increased levels of distress over a 12-month period (Harold, Fincham, Osborne, & Conger, 1997). Also, in a sample of 267 children ages 8 to 16 and their parents, the incidence of parental depression brought on by marital conflict was found to be associated with increased depression and other internalizing behaviors in children (Schudlich & Cummings, 2003).

Childrens awareness of discord in their parents relationships has been found to be related to their later psychological health. Amato and Sobolewski (2001), in a 17-year longitudinal study of 655 adult children, investigated the effects of divorce and marital discord among married couples on the psychological well-being of their adult children. Both divorce and parental discord were associated with lower levels of psychological well-being in adulthood, and the long-term implications for discord were comparable to those of marital dissolution. While the parent-child relationship served as an important mediator for later child well-being, marital discord was associated with weaker parent-child relations for both mothers and fathers. For divorce, this association was only found for fathers.  Ultimately, the quality of the relationship between parents and young adults was found to be the only significant predictor of the adult childs psychological well-being, with poorer parent-child relationships being associated with greater risks in adult offspring for distress, low self-esteem, and general unhappiness.

Children form their attitudes and beliefs about marriage in light of their experiences growing up. Childhood exposure to marital conflict has been associated with greater acceptance of divorce and cohabitation by young adults. A study of 444 young adults suggests that parental marital conflict can serve as a better predictor of childrens attitudes towards marriage than either general family conflict or parental divorce (Kozuch & Cooney, 1995).  In a study of 471 young adults whose families participated in a national longitudinal study, those who witnessed long-term marital conflict were more likely to report thinking that their parents should divorce and to hold more tolerant views of divorce, as well as being more likely to report that they did not feel accepted while living at home (Kapinus, 2005).  Particularly important were the experiences of these young adults in their teenage years. A previous analysis of these data found that it is during the late teen years that parental attitudes about marriage and divorce have the greatest effect upon the long-term attitudes of adolescents (Kapinus, 2004).

Studies consistently find that marital discord, much like divorce, affects long-term outcomes for adolescents and young adults. In addition to the individuals psychological health, experiencing marital discord also affects personal beliefs and values about divorce and how one addresses conflict in a relationship. Data from the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) suggest that young adults often replicate the family structures in which they grew up (Wolfinger, 2003) and, based on those experiences, may view marriage less positively and self-report a greater acceptance of divorce and  cohabitation (Trent & South, 1992)  These patterns were also found in a localized longitudinal study where 867 mothers who were first interviewed in 1962, were interviewed in 1985 (Thornton, 1991).

Research suggests that there is a link between negative marital interactions and negative parenting behaviors. A study of 40 married couples and their 6 to 8-year-old sons found that, following a conflicted interaction with their spouses, fathers tend to express negativity about their wives to their sons, and to remove themselves from the conversation if the mother is present (Kitzmann, 2000). The negativity of the marital interaction was related to significantly lower family cohesion, lower support and engagement by both mothers and fathers towards their children, increased family negativity, decreased family warmth, and less democratic parenting styles. Studies of 170 children ages 10 to 12 and their married parents further suggest that marital relationships affect other family relationships. Husbands have been found to be more susceptible than wives to have marital conflict spill over into problematic parent-child relationships (Brody, Arias, & Fincham, 1996). However, childrens awareness of parental conflict was associated with their negative perceptions of the mother-child relationship (Osborn & Fincham, 1996). That is, fathers were found to be more likely to act negatively toward their children because of marital conflict, yet children who were aware of conflict perceived their relationships with their mothers more negatively.

Marital conflict has been found to elicit negative, aggressive behaviors in children, in both boys and girls. Laboratory observation and questionnaire data from 156 two-parent families found that, following instances of parental conflict, adolescent girls displayed aggressive behavior patterns during family interactions, similar to those of boys and often directed at the mother (Davis, Hops, Alpert, & Sheeber, 1998). A study of 68 married-parent families and their adolescent twins provides evidence that children learn the conflict management techniques they use in parent-child interactions from observing the interactions of their parents (Noller, Feeney, Sheehan, & Peterson, 2000). However, the parent-child conflict management behaviors were not found to carry over into the sibling relationship.

A meta-analytic review of the research suggests that the parent-child relationship may not buffer children from negativity in the marital relationship, and that marital conflict has a spillover effect on parent-child relationships (Erel & Burman, 1995). The authors find that, collectively, the analyzed studies support a previous proposition that adult intimate relationships not only serve adult intimacy needs but also provide emotional support necessary for positive nurturing of children (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1988). It was hypothesized that the marital relationship exerts an effect on childrens long-term outcomes through the association of marital quality and parenting, as proposed by Belsky (1984).  Ultimately, Erel and Burman find, regardless of causality, positive parent-child relations are less likely to exist when the marital relationship is troubled (1995, p. 129). They also suggest that improving parent-child relationships may be aided through efforts to address parental marital difficulties and strengthen the skills needed to prevent difficulties from becoming profound and irreconcilable.

Booth and Amato (1994) found that, in a national sample, children who are close to one parent tend to be close to the other parent as well, when parental marital quality is high. However, among the 419 families interviewed in this longitudinal study, when parents had low parental marital quality, children tended to be close to one parent only. Marital unhappiness also appeared to weaken parent-child relationships in later life; and poor marital quality was found to affect the father-child relationships more strongly than relationships with mothers. Finally, strong parental support of adolescent children was found to mediate the association between parental marital quality, or divorce, on later parent-child relationships.

Buehler and Gerard (2002), using an NSFH subsample of 2,541 parents with a child aged 2 to 18 living with them, found both direct and indirect effects of marital conflict on child and adolescent outcomes.  In addition to directly impacting children, increases in marital conflict were found to be associated with decreased parental involvement and presence in the lives of their children. The effects of marital conflict on the parent-child relationship were found to continue even after children have left home. In a national, longitudinal study of 471 parents and their young adult children, marital conflict was associated with a lower tendency of young adults to name their parents as people who can provide them with help or assistance (Amato, Rezac, & Booth, 1995).  While it does not appear to mediate the actual exchange of assistance between parents and children, low parental marital quality seems to decrease the young-adult childs willingness to rely on parents for support.

[ Go to Contents ]

Parent Gender and the Influence of the Parent-Child Relationship

Research has well established the connection between childrens relationships with their parents and their own long-term outcomes. Often, research focuses on the influence of one parent in the absence of the other, such as in post-divorce families (Forgatch, & DeGarmo, 1997), or even on one parent to the exclusion of a present partner or spouse (Rogers, 1996; Sturge-Apple, Gondoli, Bonds, & Salem, 2003).  Studies that do address differences in parent gender often focus on parenting practices rather than on the parent-child relationship (Amato & Fowler, 2002).  For example, recent studies have investigated the connection between parenting patterns and their effects on the cognitive outcomes of young children (Ryn, Martin, & Brooks-Gunn, 2006; Smith, Landry, & Swank, 2000).  Nevertheless, a handful of studies have explored the implications of parent-child relationships by gender.

Parental gender has been associated with influence of the parent-child relationship on childrens views of divorce, particularly the quality of father-child relationships.  In a longitudinal sample of 300 European American and African American boys, adolescents who felt close to their biological fathers reported feeling less likely to divorce in the future, regardless of whether or not they live with their father (Risch, Jodl, & Eccles, 2004). The same association is found for stepfathers, suggesting that a close relationship with a father-figure contributes to a lesser acceptance of divorce. This association was not found for girls, however, which may reflect the reality that girls are not typically separated from their same-gender parent by divorce.

In a study of 325 married-parent families with sixth grade children, researchers found that mothers and fathers differed in their effect on antisocial behaviors in their children (Kosterman, Haggerty, Spoth, & Redmond, 2004).  Mothers were found to significantly affect their sons by promoting pro-social beliefs, but they did not influence their daughters. Fathers, on the other hand, directly affected the behavior of their daughters but not their sons. However, fathers had an indirect influence on their sons outcomes as paternal bonding was associated with boys having more pro-social beliefs which in turn inhibited their antisocial behavior.

[ Go to Contents ]

Influence of Parental Marital Quality on Young Men and Women

Several studies have found that higher levels parental conflict are associated with different effects for boys and girls. Boys tend to show more externalizing behaviors and psychological symptoms when parental conflict is more frequent, more severe, and more aggressive, and when it involves them (Kerig, 1996; Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992). Also, while girls tend to exhibit internalizing problems, such as depression or anxiety, they also show aggressive behaviors, particularly in interactions with their fathers. One sub-study of 79 couples suggests that it is the childs perception of the opposite-gender parent that may be most predictive of childrens reports of anxiety  that is, girls reports of anxiety are related to their fathers reports of conflict frequency, severity, and aggressiveness (Kerig, 1996). A study of 169 adolescents and their married parents suggested that the mother-son relationship is most negatively affected by parental marital discord (Osborne & Fincham, 1996).

Research on marital quality suggests that boys and girls are affected differently by their parents marital quality, through differences in parent responsiveness (Sturge-Apple, Davies, Boker, & Cummings, 2004).  Based on a 1-year study of 226 married parents of kindergarten children, higher levels of marital discord were found associated with lower levels of responsiveness to boys distress, but higher levels of responsiveness for girls distress. At the same time, parental marital conflict was associated with increased childrens aversive and aggressive behaviors, which were met with increased psychological control on the part of the parents.

[ Go to Contents ]

Marital Quality in Step-Families and Biological Families

Couples who were remarried, in an NSFH subsample of 5,642 couples, were found to report lower levels of happiness in their relationships over time than once-married couples, with remarried couples who cohabited indicating the lowest levels of happiness (Skinner, Bahr, Crane, & Call, 2002).  The amount of marital conflict in remarried couples has been found to be related both to the length of time married and to the status of children in the family.  Remarried couples with step-children but no shared biological children experience less marital conflict early in the relationship, but there is an increase in conflict as the relationship continues.  In another NSFH subsample of 2,655 married couples with children, couples with only their own biological children, or with both step-children and their own biological children, experienced more marital conflict early on in their relationships, but less as the relationships continued over time, (MacDonald & DeMaris, 1995). Couples with both shared biological and step-children were found to be at no greater risk of conflict than couples with only shared biological children.

A longitudinal study of 80 couples in first marriages and 369 couples in stablized remarriages supports the previously mentioned findings regarding the levels of conflict in complex step-families (OConnor & Insabella, 1999). In comparing first marriages with remarriages that were high functioning and stable, the authors found no significant difference in reports of depressive symptoms by spouses, and that the marital qualities of the two relationship types were more similar than different.  However, the study did note that the married adults tended to focus their parental energy on their biological children, suggesting that stepchildren do not automatically gain a new parent figure when their mother or father remarries. Another 2-year longitudinal study of 127 families found that marital conflict predicted changes in childrens behavior problems, particularly when the conflict was about the children (Jenkins, et al., 2005). Children in this study were more likely to be both exposed to conflict and a source of conflict in stepfamilies than in biological families.

Vuchinich, Hetherington, Vuchinich and Clingempeels (1991) two-wave longitudinal study of 26 families found that, compared to biological fathers, stepfathers were more positive and responsive and less negative and directive towards the children in the household.  Mothers in a stepfamily, on the other hand, exhibited more negative behavior toward the children than mothers in intact families.  Despite a stepfathers positive behavior, early adolescent children, especially girls, were likely to reject their stepfathers attempts at a relationship through avoidance and withdrawal.

Hetherington (1998) also noted that for preadolescent boys, the presence of a stepfather can reduce adjustment problems; however, a stepparents presence is linked to an increase in problem behaviors for girls.  A childs temperament can moderate their adjustment as they experience this change in family and parenting behavior during the process of divorce and remarriage.  Children with difficult temperaments may be more negatively affected by the changes than children with easy temperaments.

[ Go to Contents ]

Summary and Conclusions

Within this review we have covered the following points. First, we explained how marital quality has historically been conceptualized, defined, and measured. In addition, we explained how the current measures of marital quality found within the NLSY97 map onto other strategies developed by family science researchers. We suggest in our review that it is important to theoretically separate marital qualities (or strengths) from measures of marital conflict.

We also show how the marital quality literature juxtaposes the emerging construct of marital and family strengths with the idea of family resiliency. It was proposed that understanding a strengths/resiliency perspective takes us theoretically beyond simply understanding the nature of marital quality.

The overall purpose of this paper is to show how marital quality, strengths, and/or interpersonal protective factors work to enhance the probability that children will do better among families where strengths are higher. Additionally, we reviewed the research about parental marital quality and child outcomes and showed how those two constructs are connected. Studies indicate that conflict and parents ability to resolve that conflict affect childrens levels of anxiety and self esteem, and these effects can carry over into adulthood. The quality of the parental relationship often spills over into an adults parenting behaviors, creating a second source of tension in the family. Furthermore, negative effects of parental discord are found for both boys and girls.

Many articles were identified for this review and not included because of a focus on early childhood outcomes or grade school performance, rather than adolescent and young adult outcomes. Much of the current research on marital quality and adolescent outcomes has been collected from small or localized sample populations. Comparisons are often cross-sectional, not longitudinal. And discussions of parental marital quality are mainly based on the parents report of conflict or discord. Within the current review, several of the longitudinal studies drew from the National Survey of Families and Households and the authors of a number of the other longitudinal studies shared their data sources.

The current project addresses several of the gaps identified in the research literature by focusing on the adolescents and young adults themselves and draws on a longitudinal, nationally representative data set with information from both the parents and their offspring.

[ Go to Contents ]

Bibliography

Anderson, Stephen A. & Sabatelli, Ronald M. (2007).  Family Interaction:  A Multigenerational Perspective, Fourth Edition. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Amato, P. R., & Fowler, F. (2002). Parenting practices, child adjustment and family diversity. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 703-716.

Amato, P. R., Rezac, S. J., & Booth, A. (1995). Helping between parents and young adult offspring: The role of parental marital quality, divorce, and remarriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 363-374.

Amato, P. R., & Sobolewski, J. M. (2001). The effects of divorce and marital discord on adult childrens psychological well-being. American Sociological Review, 66, 900-921

Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., Van Dulmen, M. H. M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2005). When More is Not Better: The Role of Cumulative Risk in Child Behavior Outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(3), 235-245.

Asarnow, J. R., Tompson, M., Hamilton, E. B., Goldstein, M. J., & Guthrie, D. (1994). Family-expressed Emotion, Childhood-onset Depression, and Childhood-onset Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders: Is Expressed Emotion a Nonspecific Correlate of Child Psychopathology or a Specific Risk Factor for Depression? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22(2), 129-.

Barber, B. K., & Buehler, C. (1996). Family Cohesion and Enmeshment: Different Constructs, Different Effects. Journal of Marriage and Family, 58(2), 433-441.

Barnes, G. M., Hoffman, J. H., Welte, J. W., Farrell, M. P., & Dintcheff, B. A. (2006). Effects of Parental Monitoring and Peer Deviance on Substance Use and Delinquency. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68(4), 1084-1104.

Bauman, L. J., Silver, E. J., & Stein, R. E. K. (2006). Cumulative Social Disadvantage and Child Health. Pediatrics, 117(4), 1321-1328.

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83-96.

Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Monuteaux, M. C., & Feighner, J. A. (2000). Patterns of Alcohol and Drug Usein Adolescents Can be Predicted by Parental Substance Use Disorders. Pediatrics, 106(4), 792-798.

Booth, A., & Amato, P. R. (1994). Parental marital quality, parental divorce, and relations with parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 21-34.

Booth, A., Johnson, D. R., Branaman, A., & Sica, A. (1995). Belief and behavior: Does religion matter in todays marriage? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 661-671.

Bowen, G. L., Desimone, & McKay, J. K. (1995). Poverty and the Single Mother Family: A Macroeconomic Perspective. Marriage and Family Review, 20(1/2), 115-142.

Brewster, A. B., & Bowen, G. L. (2004). Teacher Support and the School Engagement of Latino Middle and High School Students at Risk of School Failure. Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 21(1), 47-67.

Brody, G. H., Arias, I., & Fincham, F. D. (1996). Linking marital and child attributions to family processes and parent-child relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 10(4), 408-421.

Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., Flor, D., & McCrary, C. (1994). Religions role in organizing family relationships: Family process in rural, two-parent African American families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 878-888.

Buehler, C., & Gerard, J. M. (2002). Marital conflict, ineffective parenting, and childrens and adolescents maladjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 78-92.

Capaldi, D. M., & Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Co-occurrence of Conduct Problems and Depressive Symptoms in Early Adolescent Boys: Ill. Prediction to Young-Adult Adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 11(1), 59-84.

Cates, W. (1991). Teenagers and Sexual Risk Taking: The Best of Times and the Worst of Times. Journal of Adolescent Health, 12(2), 84-94.

Coie, J. D., Watt, N. F., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., & et.al. (1993). The Science of Prevention: A Conceptual Framework and Some Directions for a National Research Program. American Psychologist, 48(10), 1013-1022.

Conger, R. D., & Elder, G.H. Jr. (1994). Families in troubled times: Adapting to change in rural America. Aldine de Gryyter: New York.

Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern Families: Selected Findings from the First Decade of a Prospective, Longitudinal Study. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(2), 361-373.

Corcoran, J., Franklin, C., & Bennett, P. (2000). Ecological Factors Associated with Adolescent Pregnancy and Parenting. Social Work Research, 24(1), 29-39.

Cox, R. P., & Davis, L. L. (1999). Family Problem Solving: Measuring the Elusive Concept. Journal of Family Nursing, 5(3), 332-361.

Cunningham, M. & Thornton, A. (2006). The influence of parents marital quality on adult childrens attitudes toward marriage and its alternatives: Main and moderating effects. Demography, 43(4), 659-672.

Curtis, K. T., & Ellison, C. G. (2002). Religious heterogamy and marital conflict: Findings from the National Survey of Families and Households. Journal of Family Issues, 23(4), 551-576.

Dahinten, V., Shapka, J., & Willms, J. (2007). Adolescent Children of Adolescent Mothers: The Impact of Family Functioning on Trajectories of Development. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 36(2), 195-212.

Davis, B. T., Hops, H., Alpert., A., & Sheeber, L. (1998). Child Responses to parental conflict and their effect on adjustment: A study of triadic relations. Journal of Family Psychology, 12(2), 163-177.

Demaray, M. K., & Malecki, C. K. (2003). Importance Ratings of Socially Supportive Behaviors by Children and Adolescents. School Psychology Review, 32(1), 108-132.

DePanfilis, D. (1996). Social Isolation of Neglectful Families: A Review of Social Support Assessment and Intervention. Child Maltreatment, 1(1), 37-53.

Dornbusch, S. M., Erickson, K. G., Laird, J., & Wong, C. A. (2001). The Relation of Family and School Attachment to Adolescent Deviance in Diverse Groups and Communities. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16(4), 396-422.

Durant, R. H., Getts, A., Cadenhead, C., Emans, S. J., & Woods, E. R. (1995). Exposure to Violence and Victimization and Depression, Hopelessness, and Purpose in Life Among Adolescents Living in and Around Public Housing. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16(4), 233-237.

East, P. L. (1996). The Younger Sisters of Childbearing Adolescents: Their Attitudes, Expectations, and Behaviors. Child Development, 67(2), 267-282.

East, P. L., & Khoo, S. T. (2005). Longitudinal Pathways Linking Family Factors and Sibling Relationship Qualities to Adolescent Substance Use and Sexual Risk Behaviors. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(4), 571-581.

Eberhart, N. K., & Hammen, C. L. (2006). Interpersonal Predictors of Onset of Depression During the Transition to Adulthood. Personal Relationships, 13(2), 195-206.

Ellison, C. G., & Anderson, K. L. (2001). Religious involvement and domestic violence among U.S. couples. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40(2), 269-286.

Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 118(1), 108-132.

Feldstein, S. W., & Miller, W. R. (2006). Substance Use and Risk-Taking Among Adolescents. Journal of Mental Health, 15(6), 633-643.

Felner, R. D., Primavera, J., & Cauce, A. M. (1981). The Impact of School Transitions: A Focus for Preventive Efforts. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9(4), 449-459.

Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1999). Prospective Childhood Predictors of Deviant Peer Affiliations in Adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 40(4), 581-593.

Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1996). Alcohol Misuse and Adolescent Sexual Behaviors and Risk Taking. Pediatrics, 98(1), 91-97.

Forgatch, M. S., & Degarmo, D. S. (1997) Adult problem solving: Contributor to parenting and child outcomes in divorced families. Social Development, 6(2), 237-253.

Franklin, C., Corcoran, J., & Harris, M. B. (2004). Risk and Protective Factors for Adolescent Pregnancy: Bases for Effective Intervention. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and Resilience in Childhood: An Ecological Perspective (2nd ed.). Washington, D.C.: National Association of Social Workers Press.

Fraser, M. W., Kirby, L. D., & Smokowski, P. R. (2004). Risk and Resilience in Childhood. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and Resilience in Childhood: An Ecological Perspective. Washington, D.C.: National Association of Social Workers Press.

Fraser, M. W., & Richman, J. M. (1999). Risk, Production, and Resilience: Toward a Conceptual Framework for Social Work Practice. Social Work Research, 23(3), 131-142.

Gerard, J. M., & Buehler, C. (2004). Cumulative Environmental Risk and Youth Problem Behavior. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66(3), 702-720.

Glenn, N. D., & Kramer, K. B. (1985). The psychological well-being of adult children of divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47(4), 905-912.

Grych, J. H., Seid, M., & Fincham, F. (1992). Assessing marital conflict from the childs perspective: The childrens perceptions of interparental conflict scale. Child Development, 63, 558-572.

Gunnoe, M. L., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D. (1999). Parental religiosity, parenting style, and adolescent social responsibility. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(2), 199-225.

Hallfors, D., Hyunsan, C., Brodish, P. H., Flewelling, R., & Khatapoush, S. (2006). Identifying High School Students "At Risk" for Substance Use and Other Behavioral Problems: Implications for Prevention. Substance Use & Misuse, 41(1), 1-15.

Hamilton, E. B., & Asarnow, J. R. (1997). Social, Academic, and Behavioral Competence of Depressed Children: Relationship to Diagnostic... Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 26(1), 77-88.

Hammen, C. L., & Rudolph, K. D. (2003). Childhood Mood Disorders. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child Psychopathology (2nd ed.). New York City, NY: Guilford Press.

Harold, G. T., Fincham, F. D., & Osborne, L. N. (1997). Mom and dad are at it again: Adolescent perceptions of marital conflict and adolescent psychological distress. Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 333-350.

Hetherington, E. M., Bridges, M., & Insabella, G. M. (1998). What Matters? What Does Not? Five Perspectives on the Association Between Marital Transitions and Childrens Adjustment. American Psychologist, 53(2).

Hauser, R. M., Simmons, S. J., & Pager, D. I. (2000). High School Dropout, Race-Ethnicity, and Social Background from the 1970s to the 1990s. In G. Orfield (Ed.), Dropouts in America: Confronting the Graduation Rate Crisis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press.

Hill, S. Y., Shen, S., Lowers, L., & Locke, J. (2000). Factors Predicting the Onset of Adolescent Drinking in Families at High Risk for Developing Alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry, 48(4), 265-275.

Horwitz, A. V., & White, H.R.  (1998).  The relationship of cohabitation and mental health: a study of a young adult cohort.  Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60(2), 505-514.

Jaffee, S. R., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2003). Life With (or Without) Father: The Benefits of Living with Two Biological Parents Depend on the Fathers Antisocial Behavior. Child Development, 74(1), 109-126.

Jenkins, J., Simpson, A., Dunn, U., Rasbash, J., & OConnor, T. G. (2005). Mutual influence of marital conflict and childrens behavior problems: Shared and nonshared family risks. Child Development, 76(1), 24-39.

Kapinus, C. A. (2004). The effect of parents attitudes toward divorce on offsprings attitudes: Gender and parental divorce as mediating factors. Journal of Family Issues, 25(1), 112-135.

Kapinus, C. A. (2005). The effect of parental marital quality on young adults attitudes toward divorce. Sociological Perspectives, 48(3), 319-335.

Kaplan, D. S., & Peck, B. M. (1997). Decomposing the Academic Failure-Dropout Relationship: A Longitudinal Analysis. The Journal of Education Research, 90(Jul/Aug. 1997), 331-343.

Kasen, S., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. S. (1998). Adolescent School Experiences and Dropout, Adolescent Pregnancy, and Young Adult Deviant Behavior. Journal of Adolescent Research, 13(1), 49-72.

Kerig, P. K. (1996). Assessing the links between interparental conflict and child adjustment: The conflicts and problem solving scales. Journal of Family Psychology, 10(4), 454-473.

Kitzmann, K. M. (2000). Effects of marital conflict on subsequent triadic family interactions and parenting. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 3-13.

Kosterman, R, Haggerty, K. P., Spoth,  R., & Redmond, C. (2004). Unique influence of mothers and fathers on their childrens antisocial behavior. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66(3), 762-778.

Kolburn Kowal, A., & Blinn-Pike, L. (2004). Sibling Influences on Adolescents Attitudes Toward Safe Sex Practices. Family Relations, 53(4), 377-384.

Kozuch, P., & Cooney, T. M. (1995). Young adults marital and family attitudes: The role of recent parental divorce, and family and parental conflict. Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, 23(3/4), 45-62.

Kumpfer, K. L., & Alvarado, R. (2003). Family-Strengthening Approaches for the Prevention of Youth Problem Behaviors. American Psychologist, 58(6/7), 457-466.

Laird, J., DeBell, M., & Chapman, C. (2006). Dropout Rates in the United States: 2004 (compendium No. NCES 2007024). Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics.

Loeber, R., Pardini, D., Homish, D. L., Wei, E. H., Crawford, A. M., Farrington, D. P., et al. (2005). The Prediction of Violence and Homicide in Young Men. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 73(6), 1074-1088.

Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single parent: What hurts, what helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

McCubbin, H. I., McCubbin, M. A., Thompson, A. I., Han, S.-Y., & Allen, C. T. (1997). Families Under Stress: What Makes Them Resilient. Journal of Family and Consumer Science, 89(3), 2-12.

MacDonald, W. L., & DeMaris, A. (1995). Remarriage, stepchildren, and marital conflict: Challenges to the incomplete institutionalization hypothesis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 387-398.

Mahoney, A. (2005). Religion and conflict in marital and parent-child relationships. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 2005.

Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Jewell, T., Swank, A. B., Scott, E., Emery, E., et al. (1999). Marriage and the spiritual realm: The role of proximal and distal religious constructs in marital functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 13(3), 321-338.

Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Murray-Swank, A., & Murray-Swank, N. (2003). Religion and the sanctification of family relationships. Review of Religious Research, 44(3), 220-236.

Mahoney, J. L. (2000). School Extracurricular Activity Participation as a Moderator in the Development of Antisocial Patterns. Child Development, 71(2), 502-516.

Mandara, J. (2006). The Impact of Family Functioning on African American Males Academic Achievement: A Review and Clarification of the Empirical Literature. Teachers College Record, 108(2), 206-223.

Marmorstein, N. R., & Iacono, W. G. (2004). Major Depression and Conduct Disorder in Youth: Associations with Parental Psychopathology and Parent-Child Conflict. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45(2), 377-386.

Maschi, T. (2006). Trauma and Violent Delinquent Behavior Among Males: The Moderating Role of Social Support. Stress, Trauma & Crisis: An International Journal, 9(1), 45-72.

Meschke, L. L., Bartholomae, S., & Zentall, S. R. (2000). Adolescent Sexualityand Parent-Adolescent Processes: Promoting Healthy Teen Choices. Family Relations, 49(2), 143-155.

Miller, B. C. (2002). Family Influences on Adolescent Sexual and Contraceptive Behavior. Journal of Sex Research, 39(1), 22-27.

Miller, B. C., Monson, B., & Norton, M. C. (1995). The Effectsof Forced Sexual Intercourse on White Female Adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19(10), 1289-1301.

Nation, M., & Heflinger, C. A. (2006). Risk Factors for Serious Alcohol and Drug Use: The Role of Psychosocial Variables in Predicting the Frequency of Substance Use Among Adolescents. American Journal of Drug & Alcohol Abuse, 32(3), 415-433.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2006). Health, United States, 2006 with Chartbook on Trends in the Health of Americans (No. 76-641496). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

Noller, P., Feeney, J. A., Sheehan, G., & Peterson, C. (2000). Marital conflict patters: Links with family conflict and family members perceptions of one another. Personal Relationships, 7, 79-94.

Norton. R (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical look at the dependent variable. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 141-151.

OConnor, T. G., & Insabella, G. M. (1999). Marital satisfaction, relationships, and roles. Society for Research in Child Development Monographs, 64(4), 50-78.

Obot, I. S., Hubbard, S., & Anthony, J. C. (1999). Level of Education and Injecting Drug Use Among African Americans. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 55(1/2), 177-182.

Olson, D. H. (2000). Circumplex Model of Marital and Family Systems. Journal of Family Therapy, 22(2), 144-167.

Orthner, D. K. (1998). Strengthening Todays Families: A Challenge to Parks. Parks & Recreation, 33(3), 87-95.

Orthner, D.K. (2007). Public schools:  Building capacity for hope and opportunity.  In

Orthner, D.K. (2007). Public schools: Building capacity for hope and opportunity. In Edwards, J., Crain, M. & Kalleberg, A.L., The Road to the American Dream: Lifting Working Families out of Poverty.  New York: New Press

Orthner, D.K., Cook, P., Rose, R. & Randolph, K.A. (2002). Welfare reform, poverty, and childrens performance in school: Challenges for the school community.  Children and Schools, 24, 105-121

Orthner, D. K., Jones-Sanpei, H., & Williamson, S. (2002). Measuring Family Strengths.Unpublished manuscript, Houston, TX.

Orthner, D. K., Jones-Sanpei, H., & Williamson, S. (2004). The Resilience and Strengths of Low-Income Families. Family Relations, 53(2), 159-167.

Osborne, L. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1996). Marital conflict, parent-child relationships, and child adjustment: Does gender matter? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42(1), 48-75.

Overall, J. E., Henry, B. W., & Woodward, A. (1974). Dependence of marital problems on parental family history. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83(4), 446-450

Patterson, J. M. (2002). Understanding Family, Resilience. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58(3), 233-246.

Pearce, L. D., & Axinn, W. G. (1998). The impact of family religious life on the quality of mother-child relations. American Sociological Review, 63, 810-828.

Pfiffner, L. J., McBurnett, K., Rathouz, P. J., & Judice, S. (2005). Family Correlates of Oppositional and Conduct Disorders in Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(5), 551-563.

Reinherz, H. Z., Giaconia, R. M., Hauf, A. M., Carmola, B. A., Wasserman, M. S., & Paradis, A. D. (2000). General and Specific Childhood Risk Factors for Depression and Drug Disorders by Early Adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(2), 223-231.

Richman, J. M., Rosenfeld, L. B., & Bowen, G. L. (1998). Social Support for Adolescents at Risk of School Failure. Social Work, 43(4), 309-323.

Risch, S. C., Jodl, K. M., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Role of the father-adolescent relationship in shaping adolescents attitudes toward divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 66, 46-58.

Rogers, S. J. (1996). Marital quality, mothers parenting and childrens outcomes: A comparison of mother/father and mother/stepfather families. Sociological focus, 29(4), 325-340.

Rosenfeld, L. B., Richman, J. M., & Bowen, G. L. (2000). Social Support Networks and School Outcomes: The Centrality of the Teacher. Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 17(3), 205-226.

Rounds, K. A. (2004). Preventing Sexually Transmitted Infections among Adolescents. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and Resilience in Childhood: An Ecological Perspective. Washington, D.C.: National Association of Social Workers Press.

Ryn, R., Martin, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Is one good parent good enough? Patterns of mother and father parenting and child cognitive outcomes at 24 and 36 months. Parenting, 6(2/3), 211-228.

Santelli, J. S., & Beilenson, P. (1992). Risk Factors for Adolescent Sexual Behavior, Fertility, and Sexually Transmitted Diseases. Journal of School Health, 62(7), 271-279.

Schudlich, T. D. D., & Cummings, M. (2003). Parental dysphoria and childrens internalizing symptoms: marital conflict styles as mediators of risk. Child Development, 74, 1663-1681.

Seccombe, K. (2002). Beating the Odds Versus Changing the Odds: Poverty, Resilience, and Family Policy. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(2), 384-394.

Short, R. J., & Brokaw, R. (1994). Externalizing Behavior Disorders. In R. J. Simeonsson (Ed.), Risk, Resilience, & Prevention: Promoting the Well-Being of All Children (pp. 203-217). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brooks Publishing Co.

Silberberg, S. (2001). Searching for Family Resilience. Family Matters, 58(Autumn), 52-58.

Simeonsson, N. W., & Gray, J. N. (1994). Healthy Children: Primary Prevention of Disease. In R. J. Simmeonsson (Ed.), Risk Resilience & Prevention: Promoting the Well-Being of All Children (pp. 77-102). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.

Skinner, K. B., Bahr, S. J., Crane, D. R., & Call, V. A. (2002). Cohabitation, marriage, and remarriage: A comparison of relationship quality over time. Journal of Family Issues, 23(1), 74-90.

Smith, K. E., Landry, S. H., & Swank, P. R. (2000). The influence of early patterns of positive parenting on childrens preschool outcomes. Early Education and Development, 11, 147-169.

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and other dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 1528.

Spanier, R., and G. Lewis. Theorizing About the Quality and Stability of Marriage. In Burr, W. et al. (eds.) Contemporary Theories About the Family: Research-Based Theories, Vol. I., New York: The Free Press, 1979.

Sroufe, L.A., & Fleeson, J. (1988). The coherence of family relationships. In R. Hinde, & J.S. Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families: Mutual influences (pp. 27-47). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Sturge-Apple, M. L., Gondoli, D., Bonds, D. D., & Salem, L. N. (2003). Mothers responsive parenting practices and psychological experience of parenting as mediators of the relation between martial conflict and mother-preadolescent relational negativity. Parenting: Science and Practice, 3(4), 327-355.

Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Boker, S. M., & Cummings, E. M. (2004). Interparental discord and parenting: Testing the moderating roles of child and parent gender. Parenting: Science and Practices, 4(4), 361-380.

Thornton, A. (1991). Influence of the martial history of parents on the marital and cohabitational experiences of children. American Journal of Sociology, 96(4), 868-894.

Trent, K. & South, S. J. (1992). Sociodemographic status, parental background, childhood family structure, and attitudes toward family formation. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54(2), 427-439.

Tucker, C. J., Marx, J., & Long, L. (1998). Moving On: Residential Mobility and Childrens School Lives. Sociology of Education, 71(2), 111-129.

Vega, W. A., Zimmerman, R. S., Warheit, G. J., Apospori, E., & Gil, A. G. (1993). Risk Factors for Early Adolescent Drug Use in Four Ethnic and Racial Groups. American Journal of Public Health, 83(2), 185-189.

U.S. Bureau of the Census (2003).  Living Arrangements of Children Under 18 Years Old: 1960 Present, (Table CH-1 & P20-537). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Vuchinich, S., Hetherington, E. M., Vuchinich, R. A., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1991). Parent-Child Interaction and Gender Differences in Early Adolescents Adaptation to Stepfamilies. Developmental Psychology, 27(4).

Wagner, B. M. (1997). Family Risk Factors for Child and Adolescent Suicidal Behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 121(2), 246-298.

Wang, M. Q., Matthew, R. F., Bellamy, N., & James, S. (2005). A Structural Model of the Substance Use Pathways Among Minority Youth. American Journal of Health Behavior, 29(6), 531-541.

Werner, E. E. (1990). Protective Factors and Individual Resilience. In S. J. Meisels & J. Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention. New York City, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Werner, E. E. (1994). Overcoming the Odds. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 15(2), 131-136.

West, M. O., & Prinz, R. J. (1987). Parental Alcoholism and Child Psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 102(2), 204-218.

Widmer, E. D. (1997). Influence of Older Siblings on Initiation of Sexual Intercourse. Journal of Marriage and Family, 59(4), 928-938.

Williams, J. H., Ayers, C. D., Van Dorn, R. A., & Arthur, M. W. (2004). Risk and Protective Factors in the Development of Delinquency and Conduct Disorder. In M. W. Fraser (Ed.), Risk and Resilience in Childhood: An Ecological Perspective (pp. 209-249). Washington, D.C.: National Association of Social Workers Press.

Wilcox, B. (2002). Religion, convention and paternal involvement. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 780-792.

 Wolfinger, N. H. (2003). Family structure homogamy: The effects of parental divorce on partner selection and martial stability. Social Science Research, 32, 80-97.

Zill, N., Morrison, D. R. & Coiro, M. J. (1993). Long-term effects of parental divorce on parent-child relationships, adjustment, and achievement in young adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 7(1), 91-103.

[ Go to Contents ]

This report was produced as part of a series of reports and briefs on the effects of marital quality and parent adolescent relationships on outcomes for adolescents and young adults. The series was prepared under the direction of Linda Mellgren under ASPE contract HHSP23320045020XI06, Task Order #3 with NORC and Child Trends. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of ASPE or HHS.

The entire series can be found at http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/08/RelationshipStrengths/. Additional papers and versions of the various reports were also published in the journal Marriage and Family Review (2009), Vol.45, issue 2/3.

 

Product Type
Literature Review
Populations
Youth | Infants & Toddlers | Fathers
Program
Healthy Marriage and Responsible Fatherhood (HMRF)